Off the wire
News Analysis: Trump orders Syria strike to send message to U.S. enemies: experts  • 24 charged in Paraguay for crimes in political unrest  • At least six killed in northern Brazil prison riot  • Trump tells Congress Syria attack ordered to protect U.S. interest  • Sharply fewer U.S. Democrats proud to be Americans: poll  • U.S.-led airstrikes kill 15 Syrians in Raqqa province  • 1st LD: Suicide bombing rocks refugee camp near Jordanian borders  • Top EU official visits Algeria to boost partnership  • One U.S. soldier killed in Afghanistan: Official  • 1st LD: Norwegian police detain suspect as "bomb-like object" found in Oslo  
You are here:   Home

Brain's navigation found more complex than thought

Xinhua, April 9, 2017 Adjust font size:

A new study suggests that the system of grid cells, known as the brain's global positioning system (GPS), is more complicated than anyone had guessed.

While the brain needs some basic navigational instruments to get around, just like a driver in a car, researchers have found brain cells that are similar to speedometers, compasses, GPS and even collision warning systems.

However, researchers with Stanford University report this week in Neuron that human brains map out the world in a more complex way, with some of the neurons in the internal navigation systems look a lot like speedometers or compasses, many others operate flexibly, each one encoding a dynamic mix of navigational variables, like a compass that somehow transforms into a GPS when driving downtown.

The project began in 2014, when Lisa Giocomo, an assistant professor of neurobiology in the School of Medicine and member of Stanford Bio-X, and Surya Ganguli, an assistant professor of applied physics, got a Bio-X seed grant to take a closer look at how the brain finds its way around. The same year, a Nobel Prize was awarded for the discovery of grid cells, specialized neurons that help animals keep track of where they are in their environments.

The findings by then were that while some neurons fell within the ballpark of how a grid cell was supposed to behave, most provided only noisy, error-prone navigation, like a GPS on the fritz. That led Ganguli, Giocomo and Kiah Hardcastle, a Stanford Neurosciences Institute graduate fellow, to wonder whether the brain had a way to correct those errors. In 2015, they reported that the brain does have a way: boundary cells, so named because they fire when nearing walls and other landmarks.

By tracking neuron firing in mice as the aminals walked around a square box, the Stanford team found that boundary cells help reset wayward grid cells, much like stumbling on a familiar spot helps reorient someone who had been hopelessly lost.

In the latest research, looking around at more navigational neurons, the research team found that only a few fit into any predefined category. "There were all these cell types that didn't have a name," Hardcastle was quoted as saying in a news release. "They weren't grid or border, head direction or speed cells, which are the four main types. This started as an extension of previous work, but then it really took a left turn."

For example, most neurons that appeared to be grid cells or head-direction cells also tracked speed. Speed cells, meanwhile, were tuned in strange ways: one cell might fire when a mouse moved either quickly or slowly, but not at intermediate speeds. Above all, it was hard to identify any particular set of neuron types, let alone a set that looked like standard navigational instruments. Instead, each neuron seemed to respond a little differently from each other.

Notifying that the team did not see grid cells or speed cells or head-direction cells, rather a big continuum, Giocomo said one of the take-home messages of the research is that there isn't a good mathematical model for the brain's navigation system. Existing models make assumptions that simply are not compatible with their results. "We need to rethink basically what the mechanism is."

There's a broader issue, she said: the cells of the brain do not necessarily think the way we think, in which case it could be misguided to assume the brain navigates using the same tools, or speedometers, compasses, and so forth as we would. "The variables that the brain cares about may not be the same as the variables that the mind cares about. There may be a discrepancy between those." Endit